Indigenous Ecological Knowledge of a Human-Elephant Interaction in Transmara District, Kenya: Implic
Advances in Anthropology 2012. Vol.2, No.3, 107-111 Published Online August 2012 in SciRes (http://www.SciRP.org/journal/aa) http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/aa.2012.23012 Copyright © 2012 SciRe s . 107 Indigenous Ecological Knowledge of a Human-Elephant Interaction in Transmara District, Kenya: Implications for Research and Management Noah W. Sitati1*, Hellen Ipara2 1World Wide Fund f o r Nature, Nairobi, Kenya 2Moi University, Eldoret, Kenya Email: *n_wasilwa@yahoo.com Received Mary 14th, 2012 ; revised June 12th, 2012; accepted June 24th, 2012 Indigenous ecological knowledge (IEK) of the Maasai community in the context of their interaction with elephants around Masai Mara National Reserve (MMNR), Kenya is explored. Although Maasai commu- nity land sustains a huge elephant population, it is experiencing increased human-elephant conflict (HEC). Focus group discussions combined with scientifically collected data were used in assessing the relevance of IEK to elephant related ecological research. The Maasai narrated their experiences with elephants which were then formulated into hypotheses and tested scientifically by designing experiments that were monitored to prove the authenticity of IEK. Respondents had in-depth knowledge of some key ecological processes. Drunken people were more likely to be attacked by elephants, and elephant movement into ad- jacent group ranches increased with increasing wildebeest density. Elephants mainly raided ripe or mature crops while pupils within the elephant range performed poorly in national examinations. Based on this, there is strong evidence that IEK could be used to design sustainable conservation strategies. It is recom- mended that understanding of IEK in mitigating HEC and its subsequent integration into HEC decision support system is necessary in order to resolve conflicts. Keywords: Indigenous Knowledge; Masai Mara; Human-Elephant Conflict Introduction The semi-arid savannah ecosystem in Eastern Africa sup- ports a large population of the African elephant (Loxodonta africana africana) and pastoral communities. Pastoralism is viewed as a compatible form of land-use with elephant conser- vation (Western, 1994). However, concerns have been raised over recent introduction of agriculture in the Mara ecosystem (Sitati et al., 2003) by immigrant farmers, which some Maasais are adopting. Consequently, human-wildlife conflicts have ensued and threaten livelihoods and wildlife conservation (Si- tati et al., 2003; Kamonjo et al., 2007). The conservation of savannah ecosystems is therefore becoming a priority for con- servation organisations (WWF, 2007). Understanding the new challenge of conflict and putting in place the necessary mitiga- tion measures are crucial for future survival of wildlife and their habitats. Among the approaches to elephant conservation, community involvement has in recent years received widespread accep- tance, despite recent critiques and project failures (Sitati & Walpole, 2006; Parker & Osborn, 2006). This approach views local participation as a prerequisite to sustainable conservation of natural resources, and its promotion has been accompanied by a raised awareness and appreciation of IEK (Ashenafi & Leader-Williams, 2005). Globally, IEK is not only recognised for its intrinsic value, but also due to its potential instrumental value to science and conservation. Local communities have in-depth knowledge about their immediate environment and IEK has been used where baseline data are needed to improve ecological research and to design conservation programmes. Due to scarcity of scientific data, IEK may offer an alternative source of informa- tion which can be rooted in long-term ecological studies (Ashe- nafi & Leader-Williams, 2005). Despite this, indigenous knowl- edge on human-elephant interaction has neither been applied nor well-documented i n e le p hant studies. In this paper, we report results from an assessment elephant related Maasai IEK with respect to ecological processes and elephant-environment relationships. Methods Transmara lies in the south-west of Kenya on the border with Tanzania, and encompasses the western portion of MMNR. The district supports 540 - 820 residents and migratory elephants on community land (Sitati, 2007), while Narok supports over 2000 elephants (KWS, 2006) some of which move up the escarpment out of MMNR seasonally and cause conflicts like crop raiding, property damage, and stopping children from going to school among others (Sitati et al., 2003). Traditionally, over 25,000 affected Maasais attempt to mitigate the conflict. Assessment of Indigenous Ecological Knowledge Twenty four Maasai community members with different background experiences participated in focus group discussions and gave historical accounts of the status, distribution and be- haviour of elephants in the district, and past human-elephant *Corresponding author. N. W. SITATI, H. IPARA relationships. During the discussions, leading questions were either read to participants or asked indirectly and this elucidated more questions. Discussions were complimented with other probing techniques that depended on the responses in order to gather information that participants could not disclose openly and also to collect diverse views. Interesting responses were treated with an echo probe to allow a participant to continue talking while baiting probe was used to reaffirm what had al- ready been learned, and to elicit further what participants were reluctant to discuss. Group discussions elicited information for testing scientifi- cally among them an outline of some long term observations of human-elephant interactions based on experience, and which derived the following checklist questions: Do elephants have a high preference for mature maize? Under what circumstances are people attacked by elephants? Does wildebeest migration increase the number of elephants on community land? Do pu- pils located within the elephant range have lower mean scores in national examinations? These questions formed the hypothe- ses and were subjected to scientific investigation for confirma- tion. Elephant’s Preference for Mature Maize To test the hypothesis that elephants damaged more mature and dry maize than young and middle level maize, ten commu- nity scouts were trained to enumerate crop-raiding incidents to circumvent the problem of exaggeration of reported conflict by farmers (Siex & Struhsaker, 1999). Farmers reported elephant crop raiding incidents to their local enumerator who visited the farm and recorded details of each incident on a standard form (Hoare, 1999b) which included: level of maize maturity using four categories; “young” (maize fields without tassels), “mid- dle” (tasselled maize with immature cobs), “mature” (ready green maize), and “dry” (maize getting ready for harvesting). Data were, first, subjected to Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for normality, and then the percentage of the different maturity levels of maize was derived. The chi-square goodness of fit test was used to test whether the observed proportions for a maize maturity level differed from hypothesized proportions. Assessment of Elephant Attacks on People In order to test the hypothesis that elephant attacks on people usually occur in the morning or at night, and that drunk people are more likely to be attacked, focus group participants enu- merated the names and areas of past cases of elephant attacks on humans. The information was verified using Kenya Wildlife Service (KWS) Occurrence Books (OBs) records and District Compensation Committee documents. Families of victims were visited and interviewed and information on victims’ sex, tribe and state (whether drunk or sober), and time collected. A chi- square goodness of fit test was used to test whether the ob- served proportions for a categorical variable differ from hy- pothesized proportions while a logistic regression was used to analyse the factors that may determine the likelihood of ele- phant attack on people (Drapper & Smith, 1981). Monitoring of Elephant-Wildebeest Interaction Elephant counts and wildebeest densities were used to test the hypothesis that elephant movement between the reserve and adjacent community lands increases with increasing wildebeest density. Focus group participants identified three frequently used elephant corridors between MMNR and communal lands for monitoring elephant movements. Community scouts and KWS rangers were stationed in the corridors and recorded ele- phant numbers and time of using the corridors daily for 12 months. A monthly count of wildebeests was undertaken in MMNR using seven permanent road transects to determine the wildebeest density for 12 months. Wildebeests within a dis- tance of 200 m on both sides of the road were counted for a distance of 10 km, and the density calculated as the number of wildebeests per km2. The Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and correlations were used for analysis. Performance of Schools in National Examination Finally, to test the hypothesis that pupils from elephant range have lower mean scores than those from outside the range, a comparison between the mean scores of 96 schools in the pri- mary school national examination, both in (n = 31) and out (n = 65) of the elephant ranges was made. The schools’ mean scores for performance in national examinations were collected from the District Education Office and used to test the hypothesis that schools within the elephant range have lower mean scores in national examinations. Nonetheless, since not all the pupils within the elephant range experienced problems with elephants, the mean scores of 277 pupils who sat for the national examina- tion in 1999, both within and outside the elephant range, were obtained from 18 randomly selected primary schools. Other details collected included: the number of days absent, distance covered to school, and whether or not elephants interfered with the pupils. The mean scores for schools and pupils with respect to elephant range were compared using ANOVA. Results Elephants Preference for Mature and Dry Maize According to IEK based on group discussions, elephants have high preference for mature and dry maize, and disappear from farming areas once maize has been harvested until the next planting season. Results also revealed that a total of 329 farms were raided by elephants. Data collected on levels of maize maturity were normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smir- nov z = 4.57, p = .000). Since maize was planted in February and March, elephants did not raid farms with very young maize. In April all maize was either young or middle, while from May to September maize was mainly mature or dry. The second planting season was September and October leading to mature and dry maize from November to January. Surveys of raided farms showed that elephants attacked more mature (53.7%, n = 261) and dry maize (37.2%, n = 181) than young (3.3%, n = 16) or middle (5.8%, n = 28) maize cro ps (2 = 177.565, df = 3 , p = .000, Figure 1). Elephant Attacks on People Group discussions revealed that the majority of the people attacked by elephants were those who stayed out late in bars and in homesteads during traditional ceremonies and walked back home late. People who engaged in early morning activities also risked being attacked. A total of 56 elephant attack cases on people were recorded between 1960 and 2000. Out of 53 such cases, 17 people were reported as “drunk”, and 36 were Copyright © 2012 SciR es . 108 N. W. SITATI, H. IPARA 0 10 20 30 40 50 Young Middle MatureDry Level of maize maturity % Farms raided Figure 1. Maize maturity level and percentage composition of raided farms by elephants. “sober”. The proportion of human deaths to injuries was not varied (2 = 2.283, df = 1, .131) over time. However, the Maasai suffered more attacks (2 = 23.11, df = 1, p = .001) than non-Maasai. More males were attacked than females (2 = 25.83, df = 1, p = .001), and the six females (13%) attacked were all Maasai. Most elephant attacks on people occurred early in the morning (0600 - 0900 hr) and late in the evening (1900 - 2900 hr), while fewer (2 = 14.075, df = 2, p = .001) cases occurred during mid-day and afternoon (Figure 2). The logistic model for factors that might have determined the likelihood of an elephant attack on people produced a goodness of fit of 73.3% of observed to expected values. Results showed that of the factors examined including a person’s state, sex, tribe and time of attack, the state of the person, whether drunk or sober, was the only factor that determined the likelihood of attack (Table 1). Wildebeest Migration and Elephant Movement The IEK from group discussions revealed that seasonal mi- gration of wildebeests from Serengeti into the Mara ecosystem from July drove elephants out of the reserve into adjacent community lands. A total of 13,059 elephants were recorded moving between MMNR and community lands between July 1999 and June 2000. The composition of elephant groups using the corridors differed between months (F11,981 = 14.365, p = .000). Mixed herds dominated between August and Decem- ber while more bulls used corridors from January to July. In- creased elephant movement coincided with the end of the crop raiding season meaning that crop raiding was mainly by the resident elephant population. Elephant numbers correlated posi- tively (p = .001) w i t h wildebeest density (Figure 3). Schools and Pupil Performance in Relation to Elephant Range According to group discussions, school children were af- fected by the presence of elephants and had to report to school late and/or leave early to avoid elephants on the way. This af- fected their performance in national examinations. Five year averages of the mean scores of 96 schools were obtained for 1995 to 1999. The mean scores were normally distributed (Kol- mogorov-Smirnov z = .765, p = .601). ANOVA showed there were differences between scores and the location of schools within and outside the elephant range (F1,94 = 19.54; p = .001). There were only three boarding schools which had a high mean grade (316.39 ± 32.74, n = 3) compared to day schools (298.65 ± 59.37, n = 93). However, scores for boarding schools did Table 1. Factors determining elephant attacks on humans based on logistic re- gression. Variable B SE Wald p Constant –5.014 2.159 5.389 .0203* State (drunk or sober) 2.816 1.154 5.959 .0146** Level of signif ica nce sh ow n w ith * = p < .05, ** = p < .01. not differ from day schools (F1,94 = .263, p = .609). Finally, a boarding school within the elephant range had a mean score of 348.66 compared with the two boarding schools outside the elephant range whose mean scores were 317 and 283 respec- tively. Although pupils from the elephant range had low mean grade, the scores for 277 pupils were normally distributed (Kolmo- gorov-Smirnov z = .857, p = .454). ANOVA showed that there was a difference between other factors that may influence per- formance (mean scores) including distance from home to school (F3,273 = 10.346; p = .001); tribe (F1,275 = 12.101; p = .001); length of absenteeism (F3,261 = 7.76; p = .001); and, school location relative to elephant range (F1,275 = 7.70; p = .006). Discussion Due to past experiences with elephants, the Maasai practise intensive guarding of crops as they mature. The Maasai believe that elephants usually send a few animals to investigate the status of crop maturity and then appear in big numbers when maize is mature. Study results revealed that the levels of maize maturity determined the seasonal patterns of crop raiding (Ta- ble 1). Young and middle level maize farms were raided mainly when: 1) farms were located along elephant routes; 2) an area had young, middle and mature maize and/or 3) elephants went out to inspect the level of maize maturity. In the later case, maize maturity raiding appears explanatory and conforms to indigenous knowledge. Elephants come in contact with agri- cultural land more easily during their natural movement pat- terns and not necessarily to raid crops (Sukumar, 1989; Hoare, 1997; Osborn, 1998). However, the ability to sense the maturity level of maize has not been understood. Mature maize is pre- ferred because of its high nutritive value and higher percentage of moisture during the dry season than grass and browse (Osborn, 1998). According to Sam et al. (1997) crop raiding occurred when crops were mature during the dry season, and natural forage is in short supply. Hence mature crops provide an important dry season food source (Osborn, 1998). In contrast, Hoare (1997) reports that crop raiding did not necessarily occur when crops were mature. These contradictory findings suggest that crop raiding may either be opportunistic, implying a preference for, rather than reliance upon, crops as a source of food. Results revealed a strong correspondence between the Maasai IEK and experience of human-elephant interaction. This however, does not imply that Maasai knowledge of ecological interactions is equal to that of scientific ecology. A trained ecologist may benefit from in-depth dialogue with local people (Donovan & Puri, 2004). Based on this, participants suggested that 1) elephants are more likely to attack men than women; 2) most elephant attacks on people occurred mostly in the evening or in the morning (Figure 2); and 3) drunk people were more Copyright © 2012 SciRe s . 109 N. W. SITATI, H. IPARA Copyright © 2012 SciRe s . 110 Figure 2. Time and frequency of elephant attacks on peo p l e i n t ransmara distr ict. 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 Jan Feb March Apr May June July Aug Sept Oct Nov Dec Mean density of elephants 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 Density of wildebeest (sq.km) Elephant Wildebeest Figure 3. Relationship between wildebeest density in Masai Mara national reserve and the mean (±SE) number of elephants moving into communal areas between January and Decem- ber 2000. likely to be attacked by elephants (Sitati et al., 2003). The scientific data generated confirmed all the three suggestions as true. Migration corridors exist in most elephant ranges, and this influences elephant movement (Soule & Gilpin, 1991; Sam et al., 1997). In Luangwa Valley, elephants moved from the valley to the higher slopes of the Muchingas to get Musuku (wild fruit), or to a pool of medicated water known as Chipatala (hospital of sick and wounded elephants) (Melland, 1938). Elephants in Transmara District move up the escarpment to feed on Acacia, which is not in the northern part of the reserve and to access forest products and salt licks (Sitati et al., 2003). Elephant corridors along the escarpment in Transmara influ- enced elephant movement between the reserve and communal land especially during the migration of wildebeests. Elephants from the reserve move up the escarpment every evening and return to the reserve in the morning (Sitati et al., 2003). Ac- cording to Laws et al. (1975) and Leader-Williams et al. (1990b), elephants form large herds in situations where they feel threat- ened and these aggregation behaviour and their movement while it is dark possibly serves as a security measure on com- munity land. However, bulls have been reported to venture into risky movement (Sukumar, 1991; Thouless & Dyer, 1992) to access high nutritive food for reproductive purposes. Despite this, elephants are now losing their fear of humans (Tchamba, 1995). Although elephants are often cited as interfering with learn- ing activities (Ngure, 1995; Mwathe et al., 1998), this allega- tion has never been explored scientifically. Study results how- ever, showed that pupils and teachers lose many hours as ele- phants sometimes block all routes between home and school. Some schools also close earlier than usual, to enable children to look for safer routes back home, and sometimes adults have to escort children to school. Such factors could definitely have a negative impact on pupil performance and this probably ex- plains why schools outside the elephant range have higher mean scores than schools within the elephant range. Despite this, distance from school was the most important factor in determining pupil performance as pupils who are exhausted from a long walk cannot concentrate in class, are often absent, and do not have enough time to finish their assignments. Pupils with unfinished assignme nt s may also av oi d school. Elephants were often used as an excuse for absenteeism and late arrival in schools, which was difficult for teachers to verify. N. W. SITATI, H. IPARA According to Mwathe et al. (1998), elephants alone may not be responsible for pupils’ absenteeism. Hence the establishment of boarding schools is seen as the best mitigation measure. Additionally, some Maasais have re- sorted to having two homesteads. One homestead is located near a school so that children can attend while the other, mainly for livestock, is away in elephant areas. This study has demonstrated that the Maasai community has detailed knowledge of elephants and their ecology and interac- tion with people. The relevance of Maasai IEK to elephant research and management is threefold. First, the congruence between Maasai and scientific knowledge of elephants suggests that local experts may be consulted for rapid and reliable eco- logical assessments since local experts often recognize unique elephant characters better than external scientists. Secondly, understanding the cause of HEC shows that there is great po- tential for combining elephant conservation and mitigating of conflict and improving local livelihoods. Lastly, local concepts and understanding of ecological processes may provide valu- able inputs to ecological research, and locally rooted knowl- edge based on long-time observations may be crucial, espe- cially in the absence of a scientific base. REFERENCES Ashenafi, Z. T., & Leader-Williams, N. (2005). Indigenous common property management in the central highlands of Ethiopia. Human Ecology, 33, 539-563. doi:10.1007/s10745-005-5159-9 Donovan, D. G., & Puri, R. K. (2004). Learning from traditional knowl- edge of non-timber forest products: Penan Benalui and the autecol- ogy of Aquilaria in Indonesian Borneo. Ecology a n d S ociety, 9, 3. Hoare, R. E. (1997). The effects of interactions with humans on ele- phant populations of the Sebungwe region, Zimbabwe. Ph.D. Thesis, Harare: University of Zimbabwe. Hoare, R. E. (1999). A standardised data collection andanalysis proto- col for human-elephant conflict situations in Africa. Nairobi: IUCN African Elephant Specialist Group. Kamonjo, N. W., Sitati, N. W., Adano, W. R., Leader-Williams, N., & Stephenson, P. J. (2007). Assessing the economic costs and benefits of maize farming in elephant ranges in Transmara District, Kenya. In M. Walpole, & M. Linkie (Eds.), Mit igating human-elephant conflict: Case studies from Africa and Asia (pp. 71-82). Cambridge: FFI Pub- lication. KWS (2006). Total dry season aerial counts of elephants in the Mara ecosystem. Nairobi: KWS Unpublished Report. Laws, R. M., Parker, I. S. C., & Johnstone, R. C. B. (1975). Elephants and habitats in North Bunyoro, Uganda. East African Wildlife Jour- nal, 8, 163-180. Leader-Williams, N., Albon, S. D., & Berry, P. S. M. (1990b). Illegal exploitation of black rhinos and elephant populations: Patterns of de- cline, law enforcement and patrol effort in Luangwa Valley, Zambia. Journal of Applied Ec ol o gy , 27, 1055-1087. doi:10.2307/2404395 Melland, F. H. (1938). Elephants in Africa. London: Country L ife Ltd. Mwathe, K. M., Ruhui, J. M., Macharia, O. K., & Warutere, J. M. (1998). Naari community fence post-project: Environmental impact assessment. The social, economic and environmental impacts of an elephant barrier on a farming community in Eastern Kenya. Unpub- lished Report. Nairobi: KWS Field Report. Ngure, N. (1995). People-elephant conflict management in Tsavo, Kenya. Pachyderm, 19, 20-26. Osborn, L. (1998). Ecology of crop raiding elephants. Pachyderm, 25, 39-40. Parker, G. E., & Osborn, F. V. (2006). Investigating the potential for chilli Capsicum spp. to reduce human-wildlife conflict in Zimbabwe. Oryx, 40, 343-346. doi:10.1017/S0030605306000822 Sam, M. K., Haizel, C., & Barnes, R. F. W. (1997). Crop raiding by elephants during the 1996 harvest season in the Red Volta Valley, Upper East Region, Ghana. WWF Project Report. Gland: WWF. Siex, K. S., & Struhsaker, T. T. (1999). Colobus monkeys and coconuts: A study of perceived human-wildlife conflicts. Journal of Applied Ecology, 36, 1009-1020. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2664.1999.00455.x Sitati, N. W. (2007). Dry and wet season elephant survey in Transmara. WWF Unpublished Re p o r t. Gland: WWF. Sitati, N. W., & Walpole, M. J. (2006). Assessing farm-based measures for mitigating human-elephant conflict in Transmara District, Kenya. Oryx, 40, 279-286. doi:10.1017/S0030605306000834 Sitati, N. W., Walpole, M. J., Smith, R. J., & Leader-Williams, N. (2003). Predicting spatial aspects of human-elephant conflict. Jour- nal of Applied Ecology, 40, 667-677. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2664.2003.00828.x Soule, M. E., & Gilpin, M. E. (1991). The theory of wildlife corridor capability. In A. Denis, & J. H. Richard (Eds.), Nature conservation 2: The role of corridors. Surrey Beatty & Sons. Sukumar, R. (1991). The management of large mammals in relation to male strategies and conflict with people. Biological Conservation, 55, 93-102. doi:10.1016/0006-3207(91)90007-V Sukumar, R. (1989). The Asian elephant: Ecology and management. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Tchamba, M. N. (1995). The problem elephants of Kaele: A challenge for elephant conservation in northern Cameroon. Pachyderm, 1, 26- 33. Thouless, C. R., & Dyer, A. (1992). Radio-tracking of elephants in Laikipia District, Kenya. Pachyderm, 15, 34-39. Western, D. (1994). Elephant and people: Statement by the director of KWS. Swara, 29, 4-5. WWF (2007). Species action plan for conservation and management of African elephants. Gl and : WWF International. Copyright © 2012 SciRe s . 111 |
上一篇:Mapping Three-Dimensional Dens 下一篇:Mobile and Terrestrial but Fir
我要分享到:
最新文章NEWS
- Soft Tissue Contributions to Pseudopathology of Ribs
- Genotoxicity Assessment of Birch-Bark Tar—A Most Versatile Prehistoric Adhesive
- Mapping Three-Dimensional Density Patterns for Analyzing Artefact (Re)distribution Trends in Palaeol
- Ancient History of the Arbins, Bearers of Haplogroup R1b, from Central Asia to Europe, 16,000 to 150
- Re-Examining the "Out of Africa" Theory and the Origin of Europeoids (Caucasoids) in Light
- The Origin of Kurds
- There Are <i>ponoks</i>, and There Are <i>ponoks</i>: Traditional Religious
- Extirpolation of the Mythology that Porotic Hyperostosis is Caused by Iron Deficiency Secondary to D
推荐期刊Tui Jian
- Chinese Journal of Integrative Medicine
- Journal of Genetics and Genomics
- Journal of Bionic Engineering
- Chinese Journal of Structural Chemistry
- Pedosphere
- Nuclear Science and Techniques
- 《传媒》
- 《哈尔滨师范大学自然科学学报》
热点文章HOT
- The Ancient Egyptian Hieroglyphic Language Was Created by Sumerian Turks
- Integrating Biomarkers into Research with Latino Immigrants in the United States
- Future Options of the Kurds.1Part I: The Current Situation
- Genotoxicity Assessment of Birch-Bark Tar—A Most Versatile Prehistoric Adhesive
- Could Wading in Shallow Water Account for the Unique Shape of the Australopithecus afarensis Pelvis?
- The Effect of Working Place on Worker’s Health in a Tannery in Bangladesh
- Mountains and Stones in Prehistory of Humanity (Explanation of Wide Spread of Great Goddess Heart-Li
- Molecular Genetic Perspectives on the Origin of the Lyngngam Tribe of Meghalaya, India